Great ape language

Facial expressions can be used to convey a message.

Research into great ape language has involved teaching chimpanzees, bonobos, gorillas and orangutans to communicate with humans and each other using sign language, physical tokens, lexigrams, and imitative human speech. Some primatologists argue that the use of these communication methods indicate primate "language" ability, though this depends on one's definition of language. The cognitive tradeoff hypothesis suggests that human language skills evolved at the expense of the short-term and working memory capabilities observed in other hominids.

By that extension, contemporary animal cognition research leans toward not teaching apes human forms of communication, but rather observing apes in their inter-species communication.[1][2][3][4] Additionally, earlier distinguished case studies in the research field such as Nim Chimpsky and Koko have been criticized as having subjects produce results based on operant conditioning.[5][6][7]

However, there is still evidence of certain apes, such as Kanzi the Bonobo, understanding crucial aspects of human language such as grammar, extensive vocabulary and reversible sentences.[8]

Apes that demonstrate understanding

Non-human animals have produced behaviors that resemble human sentence production. Some animals in the following species can be said to "understand" (receive), and some can "apply" (produce) consistent, appropriate, grammatical messages. David Premack and Jacques Vauclair have cited language research for the following animals (but see "Criticisms of primate language research", below):

While all wild animals seem to communicate, primates communicate via autonomic behaviors and displays. Among primates, behaviors like body posture, facial expressions, vocalizations and scent production have been observed to convey information to other animals, revealing emotions or alerts about potential danger. Affiliative behaviors like grooming are used to promote group cohesion and individual status, while displays of aggression create divisions among groups.[19] Humans by contrast have developed a reliance on verbal language, a skill that may be harder for non-human hominids to replicate due to a higher degree of evolved innateness in humans. Primatologist Tetsuro Matsuzawa proposes in his cognitive tradeoff hypothesis that humans exchanged short-term and working memory for better language skills over their evolution.[20]

Use of sign language

Primate language research uses sign language and computer keyboards because non-human primate have less tongue and lower jaw control[21] and their vocal cords cannot close fully.[22][23] However, primates do possess the manual dexterity required for keyboard use.

Many animal language researchers have presented evidence of linguistic abilities in animals. Many of their conclusions have been disputed.[24][25]

It is now generally accepted[a] that apes can learn to sign and are able to communicate with humans.[26] However, it is disputed as to whether they can form syntax to manipulate such signs.

Washoe

Washoe, a common chimpanzee, was caught in the wild in 1966. When she was about ten months old, she was received by the husband-and-wife research team of Beatrix T. Gardner and Robert Allen Gardner.[27] Chimpanzees are completely dependent until two years of age and semi-dependent until the age of four. Full adult growth is reached between 12 and 16 years of age. Accordingly, the Gardners received her at an appropriate age for research into language development. The Gardners tried to make Washoe's environment as similar as possible to what a human infant with deaf parents would experience. There was always a researcher or assistant in attendance during Washoe's waking hours. Every researcher communicated with Washoe by using American Sign Language (ASL), minimizing the use of the spoken voice. The researchers acted as friends and companions to Washoe, using various games to make the learning as exciting as possible.

The Gardners used many different training methods:

  • Imitation: After Washoe had learned a couple of words, she started, like chimpanzees usually do, to imitate naturally. For example, when she entered the Gardners' bathroom, she spontaneously made the sign for "toothbrush", simply because she saw one.
  • Babbling: In this case, "babbling" does not mean vocal babbling. Instead, Washoe used untaught signs to express a desire. She used a begging gesture, which was not much different from the ASL signs "give me" and "come". (Human infants who are learning sign language often babble with their hands.)
  • Instrumental conditioning: The researchers used instrumental conditioning strategies with Washoe. For example, they taught the word "more" by using tickling as a reward. This technique was later applied to a variety of relevant situations.

The results of the Gardners' efforts were as follows:

  • Vocabulary: When a sign was reported by three independent observers, it was added to a checklist. The sign had to occur in an appropriate context and without prompting. The checklist was used to record the frequency of a sign. A sign had to be used at least once a day for 15 consecutive days before it was deemed to have been acquired. Alternatively, a sign had to be used at least 15 days out of 30 consecutive days. By the end of the 22nd month of the project, thirty-four signs had been learned.
  • Differentiation: Washoe used the sign "more" in many different situations until a more specific sign had been learned. At one point, she used the sign for "flower" to express the idea of "smell". After additional training, Washoe was eventually able to differentiate between "smell" and "flower".
  • Transfer: Although the same object was presented for each learning trial (a specific hat, for example), Washoe was able to use the sign for other similar objects (e.g. other hats).
  • Combinations: Washoe was able to combine two or three signs in an original way. For example, "open food drink" meant "open the fridge" and "please open hurry" meant "please open it quickly".

Washoe also taught other chimpanzees, such as Loulis, some ASL signs without any help from humans.

Nim Chimpsky

Linguistic critics challenged the animal trainers to demonstrate that Washoe was actually using language and not symbols. The null hypothesis was that the Gardners were using conditioning to teach the chimpanzee to use hand formations in certain contexts to create desirable outcomes, and that they had not learned the same linguistic rules that humans innately learn.

In response to this challenge, the chimpanzee Nim Chimpsky (whose name is a play on linguist Noam Chomsky) was taught to communicate using sign language in studies led by Herbert S. Terrace, documented in his 1987 book.

Nim was taken from his mother at a young age by Terrace and put into a household of hippie-like people who had no background with sign language, nor did they use it. Nim was treated as more of a house pet than a wild animal. During his time in the house, Nim's family gave him access to both illegal and legal substances, such as marijuana and alcohol, and did not think twice about letting him use them. Nim's experience did not begin as an observational exercise; there were no log books kept at this point in his life.

It was not until Nim was introduced to Laura-Ann Petitto that he began his journey with learning sign language. To no surprise, Nim was almost unresponsive to sign language unless there was something in it for him if he did the sign. With many observers and trainers guiding Nim in his learning of sign language, the observations gave clear insight on what the outcome truly was. The trainers noted that Nim had made over 20,000 sequences, only for Terrace to disprove that by noticing that Nim was merely repeating signs done by his trainers. This observation had Terrace believing that, in total, Nim knew about 125 signs.

After years of being a test subject, Nim became aggressive and extremely dangerous to those around him. He would attack the researchers, sending some of them to the hospital. He bit Pettito several times, which in one instance, led to her having to get 37 stitches, and he nearly tore off another woman's cheek. In his later years, Nim was housed at a ranch, supported by the Fund for Animals in Texas, where he had access to the interior of the house, and during one incident, a small household poodle barked at him, and was subsequently smashed to death by the chimpanzee.

Overall, the experiment done on Nim did not produce much useful information. Through all of the tests it is seen that Nim merely copied the signs shown to him. This experiment also showed that non-human primates are able to memorize the outcome of certain things and if they enjoy what they get from it, they are more likely to replicate it because their memory shows them that they can get what they want with certain signs. Due to the fact that there was little to no meaningful outcomes from this project, scientists determined that non-human primates mimic, are able to memorize things with different outcomes and they have a higher likelihood of becoming dangerous and aggressive when taken out of their natural habitat at a young age.[28]

Koko

Francine "Penny" Patterson, a student of the Gardners, in 1972 began an ongoing program to teach ASL to a lowlands gorilla named Koko. Unlike the Gardners she did not limit her English speech around Koko, and as a result Koko was reported to understand approximately 1,000 ASL signs and 2,000 English words. Her results were similar to the Gardners' results with chimpanzees; although the gorilla learned a large number of signs, she never understood grammar or symbolic speech, and did not display any cognition beyond that of a 2–3 year old human child.[29]

Approximately 72 hours of video were taken recording Koko's interactions and learning behaviors. While Koko's ability to successfully produce language has been argued among researchers, behaviors that appear to mimic speech, such as breathing heavily into a telephone or other learned physical gestures have been labeled as intentional but ultimately not communicative. From a biological standpoint, non-human primates lack the correct anatomy necessary to produce the same audible speech found in humans; however vocalizations, gestures, and expressions remain a common form used to communicate in the natural world. Koko learned and was taught to compensate for this by creating cues to emulate sounds replicating speech and through her use of visual indicators.[30]

Plastic tokens

Sarah and two other chimpanzees, Elizabeth and Peony, in the research programs of David Premack, demonstrated the ability to produce grammatical messages of token selections. The selections came from a vocabulary of several dozen plastic tokens; it took each of the chimpanzees hundreds of trials to reliably associate a token with a referent, such as an apple or banana. The tokens were chosen to be completely different in appearance from the referents. After learning these protocols, Sarah was then able to associate other tokens with consistent behaviors, such as negation, name-of, and if-then. The plastic tokens were placed on a magnetic slate, within a rectangular frame in a line. The tokens had to be selected and placed in a consistent order (a grammar) in order for the trainers to reward the chimpanzees.

One other chimpanzee, Gussie, was trained along with Sarah but failed to learn a single word. Other chimpanzees in the projects were not trained in the use of the tokens. All nine of the chimpanzees could understand gestures, such as supplication when asking for food; similarly, all nine could point to indicate some object, a gesture which is not seen in the wild. The supplication is seen in the wild, as a form of communication with other chimpanzees.[31]

A juvenile Sumatran orangutan Aazk (named after the American Association of Zookeepers) who lived at the Roeding Park Zoo (Fresno, California) was taught by Gary L. Shapiro from 1973 to 1975 how to "read & write" with plastic children's letters, following the training techniques of David Premack. The technique of conditional discrimination was used such that the orangutan could eventually distinguish plastic letter (symbols) as representations of referents (e.g., object, actions) and "read" an increasingly longer series of symbols to obtain a referent (e.g., fruit) or "write" an increasingly longer series of symbols to request or describe a referent. While no claim of linguistic competence was made, Aazk's performance demonstrated design features of language, many similar to those demonstrated by Premack's chimpanzee, Sarah.

Kanzi

Kanzi has learned hundreds of arbitrary symbols representing words, objects, and familiar people (including the generic "Visitor").

Kanzi, a bonobo, is believed to understand more human language than any other non-human animal in the world. Kanzi apparently learned by eavesdropping on the keyboard lessons researcher Sue Savage-Rumbaugh was giving to his adoptive mother. Kanzi learned to communicate with a lexigram board, pushing symbols that stand for words. The board is wired to a computer, so the word is then vocalized out loud by the computer. This helps Kanzi develop his vocabulary and enables him to communicate with researchers.

One day, Rumbaugh used the computer to say to Kanzi, "Can you make the dog bite the snake?" It is believed Kanzi had never heard this sentence before. In answering the question, Kanzi searched among the objects present until he found a toy dog and a toy snake, put the snake in the dog's mouth, and used his thumb and finger to close the dog's mouth over the snake. In 2001, Alexander Fiske-Harrison, writing in the Financial Times, observed that Kanzi was "asked by an invisible interrogator through head-phones (to avoid cueing) to identify 35 different items in 180 trials. His success rate was 93 percent."[32] In further testing, beginning when he was 7+12 years old, Kanzi was asked 416 complex questions, responding correctly over 74% of the time. Kanzi has been observed verbalizing a meaningful noun to his sister.[33]

Kanzi relies highly on the lexigrams for communication, and frequently uses them to specify where he wants to go, or an item he wants to have. He does this by expressing his goal (location or object) first, and his action (go, chase, carry, give, etc.) last. This notified researchers that Kanzi's way of communicating was different from that of spoken English, especially because Kanzi would communicate many of his action words using simple gestures. In addition, Kanzi is frequently seen linking two action words together using the lexigrams, like "I Tickle", "Chase Hide", or "Chase Bite". These word combinations are not necessarily structured in a way that humans would use spoken English, but they closely resemble lists, consisting of preferred actions, in preferred order of Kanzi's social play. Because of this inconsistency of Kanzi's use of language with the spoken English language, many question whether Kanzi's understanding of English "crosses the boundary with true language".[34]

Attempts to mimic human speech and communication

Great apes mimicking human speech is rare although some have attempted to do so, and Viki, a chimpanzee, is one of them. During the 1940s and 1950s, Keith and Catherine Hayes of the Yerkes Laboratories of Primate Biology began working with a chimpanzee named Viki in an attempt to get her to mimic human speech. After undergoing months of speech therapy, Viki became their success story. Viki learned to say the words: "mama", "papa", "cup" and "up".[35] Over the years she learned to say up to seven words. Viki was extremely intelligent and like many other non-human primates, would lead people to where she wanted to go as well as move the hands of people onto objects she wanted them to manipulate. However, she would rarely point to objects that she wanted; instead she would use signs to indicate what she wanted to do. For example, when she wanted to help with ironing she would move her hand back and forth above the ironing board. This experiment with Viki would inspire other researchers to conduct similar experiments.[34]

Question asking

Despite their impressive (although still sometimes disputed) achievements, Kanzi and other apes who participated in similar experiments, failed to ask questions themselves. Joseph Jordania suggested that the ability to ask questions is probably the central cognitive element that distinguishes human and animal cognitive abilities.[36] (However, a parrot named Alex was apparently able to ask simple questions. He asked what color he was, and learned "grey" after being told the answer six times.[37]) Enculturated apes, who underwent extensive language training programs, successfully learned to answer quite complex questions and requests (including question words "who", "what", "when", "where", and "why"), although so far they failed to learn how to ask questions themselves. For example, David and Anne Premack wrote: "Though she [Sarah] understood the question, she did not herself ask any questions – unlike the child who asks interminable questions, such as What that? Who making noise? When Daddy come home? Me go Granny's house? Where puppy? Sarah never delayed the departure of her trainer after her lessons by asking where the trainer was going, when she was returning, or anything else".[38] The ability to ask questions is sometimes assessed in relation to comprehension of syntactic structures. Jordania suggested that this approach is not justified, as (1) questioning is primarily a cognitive ability, and (2) questions can be asked without the use of syntactic structures (with the use of specific intonation only). It is widely accepted that the first questions are asked by humans during their early infancy, at the pre-syntactic, one word stage of language development, with the use of question intonation.[39]

Criticisms of primate language research

Some scientists, including MIT linguist Noam Chomsky and cognitive scientist Steven Pinker, are skeptical about claims made for great ape language research.[40][41] Among the reasons for skepticism are the differences in ease with which human beings and apes can learn language; there are also questions of whether there is a clear beginning and end to the signed gestures and whether the apes actually understand language or are simply doing a clever trick for a reward.

While vocabulary words from American Sign Language are used to train the apes, native users of ASL may note that mere knowledge of ASL's vocabulary does not equate to knowledge of ASL.

See also

Researchers

Research subjects

Notes

  1. ^ Amici, Federica; Liebal, Katja (1 April 2023). "Testing Hypotheses for the Emergence of Gestural Communication in Great and Small Apes (Pan troglodytes, Pongo abelii, Symphalangus syndactylus)". International Journal of Primatology. 44 (2): 319–340. doi:10.1007/s10764-022-00342-7. ISSN 1573-8604.
  2. ^ Amici, Federica; Liebal, Katja (26 September 2022). "The social dynamics of complex gestural communication in great and lesser apes ( Pan troglodytes , Pongo abelii, Symphalangus syndactylus )". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 377 (1860). doi:10.1098/rstb.2021.0299. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 9358312. PMID 35934967.
  3. ^ Fröhlich, Marlen; Wittig, Roman M.; Pika, Simone (1 May 2016). "Should I stay or should I go? Initiation of joint travel in mother–infant dyads of two chimpanzee communities in the wild". Animal Cognition. 19 (3): 483–500. doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0948-z. ISSN 1435-9456. PMC 4824811. PMID 26833496.
  4. ^ Hobaiter, Catherine; Byrne, Richard W. (1 September 2011). "The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee". Animal Cognition. 14 (5): 745–767. doi:10.1007/s10071-011-0409-2. hdl:10023/4445. ISSN 1435-9456. PMID 21533821. S2CID 13793972.
  5. ^ Petitto, Laura A.; Seidenberg, Mark S. (1 September 1979). "On the evidence for linguistic abilities in signing apes". Brain and Language. 8 (2): 162–183. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(79)90047-6. ISSN 0093-934X. PMID 487067. S2CID 39620186.
  6. ^ Pepperberg, Irene M. (1 February 2017). "Animal language studies: What happened?". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 24 (1): 181–185. doi:10.3758/s13423-016-1101-y. ISSN 1531-5320. PMID 27368639.
  7. ^ Neisser, Arden (1990). The Other Side of Silence: Sign Language and the Deaf Community in America. Gallaudet University Press. ISBN 9780930323646.
  8. ^ Schoenemann, P. Thomas (21 July 2022). "Evidence of Grammatical Knowledge in Apes: An Analysis of Kanzi's Performance on Reversible Sentences". Frontiers in Psychology. 13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2022.885605. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 9355523. PMID 35936253.
  9. ^ Plooij, F.X. (1978) "Some basic traits of language in wild chimpanzees?" in A. Lock (ed.) Action, Gesture and Symbol New York: Academic Press.
  10. ^ Nishida, T. (1968) "The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains". Primates 9, 167-224
  11. ^ Premack, D. (1985) "'Gavagai!' or the future of the animal language controversy". Cognition 19, 207-296
  12. ^ Gardner, R.A. and Gardner, B.T. (1969), "Teaching Sign Language to a Chimpanzee", Science 165, 664-672.
  13. ^ Gardner, R.A., Gardner, B.T., and Van Cantfort, T.E. (1989), Teaching Sign Language to Chimpanzees, Albany: SUNY Press.
  14. ^ Terrace, H.S. (1979). Nim: A chimpanzee who learned Sign Language New York: Knopf.
  15. ^ a b Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S, Rumbaugh, D.M., McDonald, K. (1985). "Language learning in two species of apes". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 9, 653-665.
  16. ^ Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S., McDonald, K, Sevcik, R.A., Hopkins, W.D., and Rupert E. (1986). "Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus)". Journal of Experimental Psychology:General 115, 211-235.
  17. ^ Patterson, F.G. and Linden E. (1981), The education of Koko, New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston
  18. ^ Miles, H.L. (1990) "The cognitive foundations for reference in a signing orangutan" in S.T. Parker and K.R. Gibson (eds.) "Language" and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative Developmental Perspectives. Cambridge Univ. Press. pp.511-539.
  19. ^ Lewis, Barry (2012). Understanding Humans: An Introduction to Physical Anthropology and Archaeology 11th Edition. Cengage Learning. p. 152. ISBN 978-1111831776.
  20. ^ {Matsuzawa, Tetsuro. (2007). Comparative Cognitive Development. Developmental Science, 10(1), 97-103.}
  21. ^ Bolles, Edmund Blair (1 October 2006). "The Human FOXP2 Gene". Babel's Dawn.
  22. ^ Beard, Robert. "Can Chimpanzees Talk?". Dr. Goodword's Office. AlphaDictionary.com.
  23. ^ Falk, Dean (July 1975). "Comparative Anatomy of the Larynx in Man and the Chimpanzee: Implications for Language in Neanderthal" (PDF). American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 43 (1): 123–132. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330430116. hdl:2027.42/37553. PMID 1098478.
  24. ^ "Animal Communication". Department of Linguistics, The Ohio State University. 1994. Archived from the original on 7 February 2008. Retrieved 21 February 2008.
  25. ^ Wallman, Joel (1992). Aping Language. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-40666-6.
  26. ^ Bauman, Dirksen (2008). Open your eyes: Deaf studies talking. University of Minnesota Press. ISBN 978-0-8166-4619-7.
  27. ^ "Meet the Family – Washoe's Biography". Friends of Washoe. Archived from the original on 9 May 2008. Retrieved 21 February 2008.
  28. ^ "Language Log » Nim: the unproject". Retrieved 13 November 2019.
  29. ^ Ristau, Carolyn A.; Robbins, Donald (1 January 1982), Rosenblatt, Jay S.; Hinde, Robert A.; Beer, Colin; Busnel, Marie-Claire (eds.), Language in the Great Apes: A Critical Review, Advances in the Study of Behavior, vol. 12, Academic Press, pp. 141–255, doi:10.1016/s0065-3454(08)60048-0, ISBN 9780120045129
  30. ^ Perlman, Marcus; Clark, Nathaniel (July 2015). "Learned vocal and breathing behavior in an enculturated gorilla". Animal Cognition. 18 (5): 1165–1179. doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0889-6. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002B-870F-C. PMID 26139343. S2CID 13689291. ProQuest 1705400699 EBSCOhost 108998387.
  31. ^ Premack and Premack, The mind of an ape.
  32. ^ Fiske-Harrison, Alexander 'Talking With Apes', Financial Times, Weekend, 24–25 November 2001
  33. ^ Sue Savage-Rumbaugh, Smithsonian magazine, November 2006
  34. ^ a b Marler, Peter (June 1999). "How Much Does a Human Environment Humanize a Chimp?". American Anthropologist. 101 (2): 432–436. doi:10.1525/aa.1999.101.2.432. JSTOR 683228. ProQuest 198206524.
  35. ^ Ekström, Axel G. (February 2023). "Viki's First Words: A Comparative Phonetics Case Study". International Journal of Primatology. 44 (2): 249–253. doi:10.1007/s10764-023-00350-1. S2CID 257046483.
  36. ^ Jordania, Joseph (2006). Who Asked the First Question? The Origins of Human Choral Singing, Intelligence, Language and Speech. Tbilisi: Logos. ISBN 978-99940-31-81-8.
  37. ^ Wise, Steven M. (2002). Drawing the Line. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books. p. 107. ISBN 978-0-7382-0340-9.
  38. ^ Premack, David; Premack, Ann J. (1983). The mind of an ape. New York, London: W. W. Norton & Company. p. 29.
  39. ^ Crystal, David (1987). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language. Cambridge. Pg. 241, 143: Cambridge University. ISBN 9780521424431.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link)
  40. ^ "On the Myth of Ape Language". 2007–2008. Archived from the original on 30 September 2015. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  41. ^ Pinker, Steven (2004). "The Language Instinct" (PDF). Retrieved 24 March 2011.

References

  • Vauclair, Jacques (1996). Animal Cognition: an introduction to Modern Comparative Psychology. Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-03703-8.
  • Hillix, W.A.; Rumbaugh, Duane (2013) [2004]. Animal Bodies, Human Minds. Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects. Springer. ISBN 978-1441934000.
  • Premack, David (2015). Intelligence in Ape and Men. Psychology Revivals. Psychology Press. ISBN 978-1848722644.

Read other articles:

Questa voce o sezione sull'argomento attori è priva o carente di note e riferimenti bibliografici puntuali. Commento: Pochissime note, in particolare molte affermazioni pesanti avrebbero bisogno di fonte per dissipare i dubbi di pov Sebbene vi siano una bibliografia e/o dei collegamenti esterni, manca la contestualizzazione delle fonti con note a piè di pagina o altri riferimenti precisi che indichino puntualmente la provenienza delle informazioni. Puoi migliorare questa voce citando le f…

Association football tournament in Qatar 2022 World Cup redirects here. For other competitions of that name, see 2022 World Cup (disambiguation). FIFA 2022 redirects here. For the video game, see FIFA 22. 2022 FIFA World Cupكأس العالم لكرة القدم 2022Kaʾs al-ʿālam li-kurat al-qadam 2022الآن هو كل شيء‎Al-ʾāna huwa kullu šayʾNow Is All[1]Tournament detailsHost countryQatarDates20 November – 18 DecemberTeams32 (from 5 confederations)Venue(s)8&…

Gathering for entertainment or commerce For other uses, see Fair (disambiguation). Funfair redirects here. For other uses, see Funfair (disambiguation). This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these template messages) This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources…

هذه المقالة تحتاج للمزيد من الوصلات للمقالات الأخرى للمساعدة في ترابط مقالات الموسوعة. فضلًا ساعد في تحسين هذه المقالة بإضافة وصلات إلى المقالات المتعلقة بها الموجودة في النص الحالي. (يونيو 2023) هذه المقالة يتيمة إذ تصل إليها مقالات أخرى قليلة جدًا. فضلًا، ساعد بإضافة وصلة إل…

Jean-Baptiste SayJean-Baptiste Say.Lahir(1767-01-05)5 Januari 1767Lyon, PrancisMeninggal15 November 1832(1832-11-15) (umur 65)Paris, PrancisKebangsaanPrancisBidangEkonomi politikMazhabLiberalisme klasikDipengaruhiRichard CantillonKontribusiHukum Say Jean-Baptiste Say (bahasa Prancis: [ʒãbatist sɛ]; 5 Januari 1767 – 15 November 1832) adalah ekonom dan pebisnis Prancis. Pemikirannya beraliran liberal klasik dan mendukung persaingan, perdagangan bebas, dan pembatasan bi…

Hydrocarbon compound Not to be confused with propene or propyne. Propane Skeletal formula of propane Skeletal formula of propane with all implicit carbons shown, and all explicit hydrogens added Ball and stick model of propane Spacefill model of propane Names Preferred IUPAC name Propane[1] Systematic IUPAC name Tricarbane (never recommended[1]) Identifiers CAS Number 74-98-6 Y 3D model (JSmol) Interactive image Beilstein Reference 1730718 ChEBI CHEBI:32879 Y ChEMBL ChE…

County in Minnesota, United States This article is about the Minnesota county. For the city with the same name, see Rice, Minnesota. This article contains too many pictures for its overall length. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please improve this article by removing indiscriminate collections of images or adjusting images that are sandwiching text in accordance with the Manual of Style on use of images. (October 2023) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) This…

Si ce bandeau n'est plus pertinent, retirez-le. Cliquez ici pour en savoir plus. Cet article ne cite pas suffisamment ses sources (avril 2024). Si vous disposez d'ouvrages ou d'articles de référence ou si vous connaissez des sites web de qualité traitant du thème abordé ici, merci de compléter l'article en donnant les références utiles à sa vérifiabilité et en les liant à la section « Notes et références ». En pratique : Quelles sources sont attendues ? Commen…

الاتصال بالشابكة مرورا بمختلف مزودات الشابكة عبر العالم مزود خدمة الإنترنت[1] أو مزود خدمة الشابكة[1] أو موفر خدمة الاتصال بالإنترنت (بالإنجليزية: Internet Service Provider اختصاراً ISP)‏ هي الشركة التي توفر لعملائها إمكانية الوصول إلى شبكة الشابكة.[2][3][4] ويرتبط مزود …

You can help expand this article with text translated from the corresponding article in German. (May 2021) Click [show] for important translation instructions. View a machine-translated version of the German article. Machine translation, like DeepL or Google Translate, is a useful starting point for translations, but translators must revise errors as necessary and confirm that the translation is accurate, rather than simply copy-pasting machine-translated text into the English Wikipedia. Co…

Commune in Provence-Alpes-Côte d'Azur, France Commune in Provence-Alpes-Côte d'Azur, FranceLamanonCommuneCastle Coat of armsLocation of Lamanon LamanonShow map of FranceLamanonShow map of Provence-Alpes-Côte d'AzurCoordinates: 43°42′10″N 5°05′13″E / 43.7028°N 5.0869°E / 43.7028; 5.0869CountryFranceRegionProvence-Alpes-Côte d'AzurDepartmentBouches-du-RhôneArrondissementAix-en-ProvenceCantonSalon-de-Provence-1IntercommunalityAix-Marseille-ProvenceGovernment…

Women's scratch at the 2023 UEC European Track ChampionshipsVenueTissot Velodrome, GrenchenDate8 FebruaryCompetitors19 from 19 nationsMedalists  Maria Martins   Portugal Eukene Larrarte   Spain Daria Pikulik   Poland← 20222024 → 2023 UEC EuropeanTrack ChampionshipsSprintmenwomenTeam sprintmenwomenTeam pursuitmenwomenKeirinmenwomenOmniummenwomenMadisonmenwomenTime trialmenwomenIndividual pursuitmenwomenPoints r…

Adachi 足立区Distrik kota istimewa BenderaLambangLokasi Adachi di Prefektur TokyoNegara JepangWilayahKantōPrefektur TōkyōPemerintahan • Wali kotaYayoi KondōLuas • Total53,3 km2 (206 sq mi)Populasi (Oktober 1, 2015) • Total670.122 • Kepadatan12,572/km2 (32,56/sq mi)Zona waktuUTC+9 (WSJ)Kode pos120-8510Simbol • PohonSakura• BungaTulipaNomor telepon03-3880-5111Alamat1-17-1 Chūōhonchō, Adachi-k…

Ginger RogersRogers pada tahun 1943LahirVirginia Katherine McMath(1911-07-16)16 Juli 1911Independence, Missouri, A.S.Meninggal25 April 1995(1995-04-25) (umur 83)Rancho Mirage, California, A.S.MakamOakwood Memorial Park Cemetery, Chatsworth, California, A.S.PekerjaanAktrispenaripenyanyiTahun aktif1925–1987Suami/istriJack Pepper ​ ​(m. 1929; c. 1931)​Lew Ayres ​ ​(m. 1934; c. 1940)​Jack Brig…

This article relies excessively on references to primary sources. Please improve this article by adding secondary or tertiary sources. Find sources: Apple speakers – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (May 2023) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Music and multimedia speakers by Apple Apple Inc. has produced and sold numerous music and multimedia speakers, available for standalone purchase and bundled with Macintosh products. Pl…

Type of infantry for leading attacks For the 1967 film, see Shock Troops (film). US Army troops getting on a UH-60 military helicopter preparing for air assault training at Nowa Dęba, Poland, 2015 Shock troopers or assault troopers are formations created to lead an attack. They are often better trained and equipped than other infantry and expected to take heavy casualties even in successful operations. Shock troop is a calque, a loose translation of the German word Stoßtrupp (literally push sq…

Music festival in Tampere, Finland Sauna Open AirGenreHeavy metalDatesJuneLocation(s)Tampere, FinlandYears active2004–20132019[1]–2022Websitewww.saunaopenair.fi Sauna Open Air Metal Festival, shortly Sauna Open Air or just Sauna, was one of the largest music festivals dedicated only to heavy metal and related styles of rock music in the Nordic countries. It was first arranged in 2004 and takes place in the Ratinanniemi park in Tampere, Finland. From 8 June to 10 June 2006, over 20,00…

Si ce bandeau n'est plus pertinent, retirez-le. Cliquez ici pour en savoir plus. Cet article ne cite aucune source et peut contenir des informations erronées (signalé en novembre 2021). Si vous disposez d'ouvrages ou d'articles de référence ou si vous connaissez des sites web de qualité traitant du thème abordé ici, merci de compléter l'article en donnant les références utiles à sa vérifiabilité et en les liant à la section « Notes et références ». Trouver des sources…

Questa voce sull'argomento anatomia è solo un abbozzo. Contribuisci a migliorarla secondo le convenzioni di Wikipedia. Segui i suggerimenti del progetto di riferimento. Le informazioni riportate non sono consigli medici e potrebbero non essere accurate. I contenuti hanno solo fine illustrativo e non sostituiscono il parere medico: leggi le avvertenze. Muscolo peroneo breveSi osserva il muscolo peroneo breveAnatomia del Gray(EN) Pagina 487 SistemaSistema muscolare Originelateral surface of …

B

  此條目介紹的是拉丁字母中的第2个字母。关于其他用法,请见「B (消歧义)」。   提示:此条目页的主题不是希腊字母Β、西里尔字母В、Б、Ъ、Ь或德语字母ẞ、ß。 BB b(见下)用法書寫系統拉丁字母英文字母ISO基本拉丁字母(英语:ISO basic Latin alphabet)类型全音素文字相关所属語言拉丁语读音方法 [b][p][ɓ](适应变体)Unicode编码U+0042, U+0062字母顺位2数值 2歷史發展…

Kembali kehalaman sebelumnya